doi:10.5477/cis/reis.190.21-42
Alcohol Consumption among Adolescents
and the Information Paradox
Consumo de alcohol entre adolescentes y la paradoja de la información
Ángel Belzunegui Eraso, Verónica Díaz Moreno, Inma Pastor Gosálbez,
Anna Sánchez Aragón, Francesc Valls Fonayet and Jorge de Andrés Sánchez
Keywords Adolescence
|
Abstract Alcohol is the neurotoxic substance that is most frequently consumed by adolescents. Thus, it is a highly relevant topic in the sociology of health given the negative consequences at this stage of development. This study, based on a survey of 1307 adolescents aged 15 to 18 in Tarragona, analyzes individual, environmental and informational factors influencing three types of alcohol consumption. Age and peer influence are found to increase the likelihood of consumption, although gender does not. Contrary to expectations, those adolescents who believe that they are better informed about the effects of consumption do not display lower consumption rates, suggesting an “information paradox”. Furthermore, it was found that information from friends facilitates consumption, whereas information from formal sources, such as the media, inhibits said consumption. |
Palabras clave Adolescencia
|
Resumen El alcohol es la sustancia neurotóxica más consumida por los adolescentes. Así, se trata de un tema relevante en la sociología de la salud debido a las consecuencias negativas en esta etapa de formación. Este estudio, basado en una encuesta a 1307 adolescentes de quince a dieciocho años en Tarragona, analiza factores individuales, ambientales e informativos que influyen en tres modalidades de consumo de alcohol. La edad y la influencia de los pares aumentan la probabilidad de consumo, pero no el género. Contrario a lo esperado, aquellos adolescentes que se sienten mejor informados sobre los efectos del consumo no presentan menores tasas de consumo, revelando una «paradoja de la información». Además, se encontró que la información de amigos facilita el consumo, mientras que la información de fuentes formales, como medios de comunicación, inhibe dicho consumo. |
Citation
Belzunegui Eraso, Ángel; Díaz Moreno, Verónica; Pastor Gosálbez, Inma; Sánchez Aragón, Anna; Valls Fonayet, Francesc; Andrés Sánchez, Jorge de (2025). “Alcohol Consumption among Adolescents and the Information Paradox”. Revista Española de Investigaciones Sociológicas, 190: 21-42. (doi: 10.5477/cis/reis.190.21-42)
Ángel Belzunegui Eraso: Universitat Rovira i Virgili | angel.belzunegui@urv.cat
Verónica Díaz Moreno: Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia | vdiaz@poli.uned.es
Inma Pastor Gosálbez: Universitat Rovira i Virgili | inma.pastor@urv.cat
Anna Sánchez Aragón: Universitat Rovira i Virgili | annamaria.sanchez@urv.cat
Francesc Valls Fonayet: Universitat Rovira i Virgili | francesc.valls@urv.cat
Jorge de Andrés Sánchez: Universitat Rovira i Virgili | jorge.deandres@urv.cat
Introduction1
Adolescence is a crucial phase in the development of individuals, during which alcohol consumption becomes a problem, according to the State Survey on Drug Use in Secondary Education (OEDA, 2022; Leal-López, 2021a). During this period, the neurotoxic effects of alcohol may have serious health consequences, causing physical problems (Wellman, Sabiston and Morgenstern, 2022), alterations in brain development and neurocognitive difficulties (Tong et al., 2022). Furthermore, it may lead to a decline in academic performance, high-risk sexual behavior, accidents (Cabrera et al., 2022) and peer harassment behavior (Prignitz et al., 2023). These findings justify the classification of alcohol abuse as one of the main threats to adolescent environmental health (Ortega-García et al., 2019).
Although alcohol consumption by youth has demonstrated a downward trend since the late 20th century, both in Spain (OEDA, 2022) and internationally (Gual and Colom, 1997; Leal-López et al., 2021b), alcohol continues to be the most frequently consumed psychoactive substance by adolescents (OEDA, 2022). According to data from the Spanish Observatory on Drugs and Addictions, 54.1 % of all sixteen year old students have experienced alcohol poisoning; almost three out of ten have engaged in binge drinking2 over the past month; and 20.8 % have gotten drunk during this same period (OEDA, 2023), with weekend consumption being especially intensive (Calafat et al., 2005; Cortés, Espejo and Giménez, 2007; Sánchez-Quejica et al., 2015).
During the adolescent period, social acceptance and environmental availability of alcohol favor access to its consumption (Sánchez-Aragón et al., 2024). For this reason, over recent years, the implementation of preventive strategies has intensified, many of which focus on informing adolescents about the risks of drug abuse through school programs (Suárez et al., 2014). In fact, 29.6 % of the students affirm that they are fully informed about drug consumption (OEDA, 2023). This information, however, has not led to lower consumption rates, a phenomenon that has been labelled by some authors as “the information paradox” (Belzunegui-Eraso et al., 2020).
Currently, little is known regarding how information acts on substance use. Therefore, the objective of this work is to observe the effects of information sources used by adolescents on alcohol consumption and its most extreme forms. At the same time, it is interesting to determine how the influence of certain individual and contextual variables on consumption is modulated, once the sources of information have been introduced.
Some factors associated with alcohol consumption
Scientific literature has suggested that substance use should be understood by considering the ecological context in which adolescents are socialized, taking into account both the social groups to which they belong and the influence of these on their beliefs, values and attitudes (Pons and Buelga, 2011). This approach examines the role of risk factors and protection factors.
Studies analyzing the influence of socialization agents have confirmed the role played (at a microsocial level) by the family on the minor’s relationship with psychoactive substances. Evidence suggests that a lack of parental control, permissiveness in education and inconsistency in establishing standards are factors that increase the likelihood of drug use (Jiménez-Iglesias et al., 2013; Cruz-Salmerón et al., 2011). Specifically, a lack of adequate parental supervision has been associated with increased substance use (Guo et al., 2011; Perelló, Llorens, and Tortajada, 2008). Therefore, both a neglectful parenting style as well as an authoritarian approach, characterized by rigid and arbitrary rules, act as risk factors (Mehanović et al., 2022; Berge et al., 2016). In contrast, a caring and democratic parenting style acts as a protective factor (Novak, Maglica and Radetic Paic, 2022; Fuentes et al., 2015; Wray-Lake, Crouter and McHale, 2010).
Parental behavior regarding substance use also has a significant influence. Evidence shows that adolescents who grow up in homes where drugs are used tend to develop a more favorable attitude toward them and a lower perception of risk, thus increasing their likelihood of using drugs (Obradors-Rial, Ariza and Muntaner, 2014; Urrutia-Pereira et al., 2019).
Although the family model has a prominent influence in early adolescence (Brown, 2008), the group of friends becomes the main influencing factor in alcohol consumption (Helmer et al., 2021; Neighbors et al., 2016). In Spain, according to data from the national survey on drug use in young people, 80.4 % of the students whose friends (all or the majority) drank alcoholic beverages during the past thirty days had also consumed alcohol during this period. In contrast, only 39.6 % of those students in groups with few or no friends who drink alcohol reported drinking it themselves (OEDA, 2022).
Friendships are crucial for the initiation and maintenance of drug use, since sharing these habits facilitates social integration and decreases the risk of rejection (Gommans et al., 2017; Calero et al., 2022). There are diverse reasons for drinking: disconnecting from everyday problems, relaxing (Smit et al., 2022; Patrick et al., 2024) or, in other cases, escaping from emotions such as stress, anxiety, sadness, nervousness or loneliness (Cano et al., 2012).
Of the personality characteristics, young people presenting traits such as emotional instability, low self-esteem, impulsiveness or low tolerance to frustration are more vulnerable to consumption (Melguizo-Ibáñez et al., 2023). These adolescents are often intolerant to boredom and demonstrate a constant need for stimuli, leading them to seek out new and riskier experiences (Pérez de Albéniz-Garrote, Medina-Gómez and Rubio-Rubio, 2019). Other factors playing a significant role in substance use are irritability and the questioning of social norms. Irritability is bidirectionally related to alcohol use, since increased irritability may facilitate consumption and, in turn, alcohol consumption may trigger irritable behaviors (Nawi et al., 2021). In addition, the tendency to question the rules, including those regarding drug consumption, may facilitate the initiation of consumption (Bousoño et al., 2021). Similarly, alcohol consumption may encourage antisocial behavior, creating a cycle of mutual influence (Cabrera et al., 2022; Prignitz et al., 2023).
Religious practices influence adolescents’ behaviors and attitudes, promoting healthy lifestyles and strengthening resistance to risky behaviors (Afifi et al., 2020; Andrés-Sánchez and Belzunegui-Eraso, 2022). In studies focused on Spain, strict rules prohibiting the use of alcohol explain the special capacity of the Islamic religion to protect against its consumption (Charro-Baena et al., 2019). As for sex, the ESTUDES survey (OEDA, 2023) revealed the growing tendency of girls to be included in legal drug consumption, exceeding boys in alcohol consumption. One illustrative fact is the participation of girls in the practice of botellón (drinking in street parties) in percentages exceeding that of boys (OEDA, 2023).
Along with these individual factors, risk perception also plays a crucial role in the initiation of substance use. In general, when young people perceive that the associated risk is low, the likelihood of consumption increases (OEDA, 2023). Although adolescents receive numerous messages about the negative effects of drug use, such as addiction and dependence, through various channels, a carefree attitude often prevails. This is related to what is referred to as the “illusion of invulnerability”, a characteristic of adolescent thought (Mietzel, 2005). This is a type of perception of reality that leads the individual to believe that the negative consequences of substance use can only occur to others (Lapsley and Hill, 2010). Some studies suggest that this belief contributes to the fact that, despite being informed, adolescents continue to consume substances (Pons and Buelga, 2011), which has been called the “information paradox” (Belzunegui-Eraso et al., 2020), as previously mentioned.
According to the data collected in the ESTUDES survey (OEDA, 2022), amongst the drug consumption behaviors that students aged 14-18 associated with a lower risk were drinking 5 or 6 beers or glasses of alcoholic beverages on the weekend (56.5 %). Legally sold drugs (alcohol, tobacco and hypnotics/sedatives) are perceived as being less dangerous than illegal substances other than cannabis. In all consumer practices, with a few exceptions, the perception of risk increases with age, with the feeling of danger being greatest in the eighteen-year age group. Some studies have also found significant differences based on personal consumption, with alcohol consumers perceiving it as less dangerous than non-consumers (Salamó-Avellaneda, Gras-Pérez and Font-Mayolas, 2010).
García del Castillo, García del Castillo-López and López-Sánchez (2014) drew attention to the fact that the information received by young people about alcohol consumption is often contradictory. On the one hand, publicity campaigns associate alcohol consumption with pleasant experiences, accompanied by the message “enjoy with responsible consumption”, which seeks to promote moderate consumption. On the other hand, prevention programs in schools often focus on messages based on appeals to fear. Musitu (2014) argued that the preventive discourse on the harms of alcohol clashes with the so-called “vitalistic presenteeism” that characterizes adolescents (Megías et al., 2006). On the other hand, taking a more generic approach, Martínez-Oró and Romaní (2016) criticized the prohibitionist orientation that has led to the stigmatization of consumers with unintended consequences, including discrimination.
Cross-sectional and correlational study based on a questionnaire administered to secondary school students in Tarragona, whose demographic characteristics are presented in Table 1. The survey was conducted in February-March of 2023 and was addressed to adolescents in the final course year of compulsory secondary education, baccalaureate studies or in professional training courses. The questionnaire was completed online under supervision. The sample size was N=1307 and the margin of error was 2.6 ٪. It was found that 46.5 ٪ of the responses corresponded to girls and 51.2 ٪ to boys, aged between fifteen and eighteen years (2.3 ٪ did not respond). The mean age was 16.4 (SD=0.96).
Three specific hypotheses were formulated:
H1 affirms the existence of statistically significant differences in alcohol consumption, depending on the age and sex of the adolescents.
H2 expresses that the influence of peers stands out as a risk factor for alcohol consumption and its most intensive forms.
H3 considers that information on substance use based mainly on unsupervised sources is a facilitator of alcohol consumption.
Definition of the variables and the measures
The dependent variables of this study are the modalities of alcohol consumption (see List 1).
The explanatory factors were defined from variables classified as individual (sex, age, normative conformity and irritability), environmental (school, family and friends) and information (perception of information quality and source of information) variables (see List 2). Table 1 shows the frequencies of the items included in these constructs.
Statistical analysis
In an initial step, the reliability of the scales with two factors was measured: IRRITABILITY, SCHOOL SUPPORT, CONTROL_P and INFL_PEERS. Conventional measures were used, such as Cronbach’s alpha (α), composite reliability (CR), average variance extracted (AVE) and the factorial loads of the indicators for each latent variable. The scales are considered reliable if α and CV > 0.7, AVE > 0.5, and the factorial loads are less than 0.6.
Subsequently, a correlation analysis was performed between the alcohol consumption variables and their explanatory variables. Given the ordinal nature of the variables, the Spearman Rho correlation was calculated (ρ), which is a nonparametric test. For IRRITABILITY, SUPPORT_SCHOOL, CONTROL_P and INFL_PEERS, the factorial scores were considered as values. Dichotomous variables were used as dummy variables (Suites, 1957 [2012]).
In addition, three ordered logistic regressions were performed to examine the relationship between CONSUMPTION, DRUNKENNESS and BINGE and the individual environmental and informative factors. These regressions were created in two stages. Initially, individual and environmental factors were introduced to assess their relevance to alcohol consumption habits. Then, the variables linked to the information were included.
In addition to the conventional analysis of the significance of the regression coefficients, the values of the Akaike information criterion, the Schwartz criterion and the Hannan-Quinn criterion were compared between the model that excludes the information-related variables and the model that includes them. The inclusion of informative variables resulted in a decrease in the Akaike, Schwartz and Hannan-Quinn measures, which is considered positive in the evaluation of the model, as shown in Tables 3 to 5.
Descriptive analysis of the variables and reliability of the scales
Regarding the prevalence of consumption, Table 1 reveals that 38.4 ٪ of all adolescents report having consumed alcohol over the past thirty days. Similarly, 17.3 ٪ report having gotten drunk during the past month and 22.5 ٪ indicate that they have engaged in binge drinking over the past thirty days.
Regarding the level of information declared by the adolescents surveyed, 36.7 ٪ consider themselves very informed and 42.1 ٪ fully informed about substance use. As for the sources of information, the majority declare that they are informed at school (66.3 ٪), over the Internet (65.9 ٪) and by their parents or legal guardians (62.4 ٪). Fifty five percent (55 ٪) are informed over the mainstream media, 44.7 ٪ by their peers and 23.4 ٪ by their siblings.
The scales revealed consistency, especially in the case of parental control, peer influence and irritability, in which the factor loadings of the indicators exceed 0.6 and even 0.7. The values of Cronbach’s alpha (α) and composite reliability (CR) are both above 0.7, while the average variance extracted (AVE) exceeds 0.5. On the scale of regulatory compliance, the AVE is slightly less than 0.5. However, despite the fact that these are not optimal results, the scale’s consistency can be accepted since the CR is significantly greater than 0.6 (Lam, 2012).
A similar situation is observed in the case of perceived school support. The low AVE value (0.49) is compensated for by the high-observed values of Cronbach’s alpha (α) and the composite reliability (CR). The factorial loads of this latent variable are adequate, given that all of them are greater than 0.6. According to Hair and Alamer (2022), this value may be justified by the good results obtained in the α and CR.
Correlational analysis between explanatory factors and consumption variables
As for individual factors, bivariate analysis reveals that age and irritability correlate with all of the examined forms of alcohol consumption (see Table 2). The CU_RULES also reveals a significant positive correlation with CONSUMPTION and with BINGE, and a more moderate correlation with DRUNKENNESS. Sex does not display statistically significant differences with the alcohol consumption modalities.
As for the environmental variables, the influence of peers and being Muslim are the most relevant factors. The association between INFL_PEERS and the alcohol consumption items is positive, with correlations approaching 0.2 (p<0.001). Identifying with Islam is (significantly) negatively related to all three dependent variables.
Regarding the correlation between parental control and the distinct consumption modes, the correlations are not significant except for the drunkenness consumption mode. As expected, school support shows a negative correlation with consumption, but this relationship also fails to be significant.
The variables related to information on substance use offer some interesting results. On the one hand, the perceived level of information on substance use does not correlate significantly with the different forms of alcohol consumption.
When the information comes mainly from friends or siblings, statistically significant correlations with consumption exist. However, when the information comes from the Internet, it correlates significantly with CONSUMPTION and DRUNKENNESS.
It should also be acknowledged that significant correlations do not result with regard to obtaining information mainly through the school, parents and the media.
Regression analysis of consumption variables
Tables 3, 4 and 5 show the results of the adjustments for CONSUMPTION, DRUNKENNESS and BINGE DRINKING, respectively, using ordered logit regressions. It is seen that the adjustments for the three types of alcohol consumption, without the inclusion of information-related variables, provide statistically significant models.
However, in all types of consumption, including variables related to the information available to the adolescent provides a better quality fit. The maximum likelihood ratios increase, with p<0.001, and, similarly, the information criteria of Akaike, Schwartz, and Hannan and Quinn of the models always decrease, leading to the selection of the model that includes the variables on information.
As for the individual variables, as expected, sex does not present statistical significance. On the other hand, age appears to be the most relevant variable, having a positive relationship with the consumption variables in all cases.
Non-compliance with standards appears to be a facilitator of consumption in CONSUMPTION (OR=1.185, p=<0.001) and in BINGE (OR=1.183, p=0.001), but not for DRUNKENNESS. Irritability is a risk factor in all types of alcohol consumption (CONSUMPTION, OR=1.123, p=0.007; DRUNKENNESS, OR=1.246, p<0.001; BINGE, OR=1.126, p=0.014).
Regarding the environmental variables, parental control suggests that it is possible to have a significant protective factor against parental consumption in the form of DRUNKENNESS (OR=0.843; p=0.036) and BINGE DRINKING (OR=0.859; p=0.040). Support from the school only demonstrates a protective capacity with regard to DRUNKENNESS (OR=0.896; p=0.045).
Without a doubt, of the environmental factors considered, the two most relevant ones in terms of alcohol consumption are peer influence, which acts as a facilitating element (CONSUMPTION: OR=1.234; p<0.001); DRUNKENNESS: OR=1.240; p<0.001); BINGE: OR=1.321; p<0.001). Being Muslim is a strong inhibitor (CONSUMPTION: OR=0.134; p<0.001); DRUNKENNESS: OR=0.250; p<0.001); BINGE: OR=0.212; p<0.001).
The level of information that adolescents report having about the consequences of substance use does not influence how they consume alcohol. No statistically significant differences exist between those who consider themselves well informed and those who do not.
Information from schools and parents may have a certain protective capacity against alcohol consumption, since they have an OR<1, but it is not significant. Likewise, the odds ratio reached with information from the Internet suggests that this source of information may act as an enhancer of all of the alcohol consumption forms examined in this work. However, it also does not reach statistical significance.
Information from the conventional media has a significant protective effect against excessive alcohol consumption (DRUNKENNESS: OR=0.774; p=0.022); BINGE: OR=0.729; p=0.002). On the other hand, information from siblings, and especially from peers, has a clear facilitating effect on all the types of alcohol consumption analyzed.
Without a doubt, the information obtained from peers is the source having the greatest impact on all of the consumption modalities. Therefore, in CONSUMPTION, its OR=1.422 (p<0.001), in DRUNKENNESS, OR=1.542 (p<0.001), and in BINGE, OR=1.621 (p<0.001).
The literature has documented that a statistically significant correlation exists between age and consumption patterns, which increase as adolescents grow older (De Looze et al., 2017; López-Larrosa and Rodríguez-Arias, 2010). Likewise, adolescents displaying higher levels of irritability have a higher prevalence of alcohol consumption, as confirmed by works of Méndez-Garrido and Azaustre-Lorenzo (2016), among others. The failure to comply with regulations also correlates with higher consumption, although in a more moderate way, as reflected in the work of Rubio et al. (2022).
More parental control and support acts as a protective factor against more extreme alcohol consumption, as shown by Berge et al. (2016) and García-Barba et al. (2018). On the contrary, the influence of peers presents positive correlations with consumption, as described by Pons and Buelga (2011), Talbott et al. (2008), Song et al. (2012) and Burk et al. (2012). On the other hand, being Muslim correlates negatively, as King and Roeser (2009) and Andrés and Belzunegui (2022) revealed. And no correlations have been found between consumption and other religious identifications.
The vast majority of adolescents (approximately 80 ٪) consider themselves well informed with regard to substance use. However, the lack of correlation between the level of perceived information and alcohol consumption suggests that this perception is not a determining factor in explaining the different degrees of alcohol consumption. This finding suggests the need to evaluate the origin of such information to determine whether it influences consumption patterns (Belzunegui et al., 2020) and the underestimation of the associated risks, in line with the findings of García del Castillo, García del Castillo-López and López-Sánchez (2014).
Despite having an OR<1, the information provided by the centers and parents does not have sufficient statistical significance to be considered a protective factor against alcohol consumption. Similarly, the information provided by the Internets, despite having an OR>1, also fails to be significant. Since these are the main sources of information for adolescents, their lack of significance calls into question the sources of information for prevention models, a criticism that has already been mentioned in other studies. Villanueva and Duque (2022: 92), after reviewing multiple school intervention programs, concluded that:
Interventions based on the transfer of information are not effective, beyond merely increasing knowledge. Information on its own does not lead to changes in behavior.
Information from conventional media, which is assumed to be supervised information, may act as a protective factor against excessive alcohol consumption if it presents well-informed and non-alarmist messages, as suggested by Córdoba, Camarelles and San José (2017). On the other hand, certain messages minimize the risk of consumption, giving the conventional media a contradictory role. However, the information coming from siblings, and especially that from peers and friends, acts as a facilitator of alcohol consumption, suggesting the importance of the influence of social models (Mezquita et al., 2006).
This analysis has confirmed the effect of different factors such as age, peer influence and information on substance use from informal channels, as predictors of alcohol consumption. Furthermore, irritability has been found to increase the probability of consumption, whereas parental control has been shown to have a protective effect against some of the more aggressive forms of this behavior, such as drunkenness. On the other hand, it has been concluded that sex, support from the educational center or formal information channels are not related to consumption.
It has also been found that alcohol consumption amongst adolescents is independent of the degree to which they consider themselves informed. This topic should be examined in future research, focusing on the quality of information that is handled by the individuals. Although adolescents report being well informed about the effects of alcohol, this perception fails to reduce their consumption levels. In fact, information from friends and siblings acts as a facilitator of consumption, while information from formal sources such as the media or educational centers may inhibit consumption, but not in a statistically significant way. This finding may be relevant to the organization and planning of alcohol consumption prevention programs.
In summary, although adolescents are increasingly informed of the dangers of alcohol, this information alone fails to reduce their consumption. Therefore, preventive strategies should focus not only on information dissemination, but also on addressing social influence and other personal and contextual risk factors (especially social class and occupational status of parents).
Afifi, Rima A.; Asmar, Khalil; Bteddini, Dima; Assi, Moubadda; Yassin, Nasser; Bitar, Sara and Ghandour, Lilian (2020). “Bullying Victimization and Use of Substances in High School: Does Religiosity Moderate the Association?”. Journal of Religion and Health, 59: 334-350. doi: 10.1007/s10943-019-00789-8
Andrés-Sánchez, Jorge de; Belzunegui-Eraso, Ángel and Fernández-Aliseda, Sonia (2022). “Religion as a Protective Factor Against Adolescent Smoking Habits: Evidence from Spain”. Christian Journal for Global Health, 8(2): 16-23. doi: 10.15566/cjgh.v8i2.579
Belzunegui-Eraso, Ángel; Pastor-Gosálbez, Inma; Raigal-Aran, Laia; Valls-Fonayet, Francesc; Fernández-Aliseda, Sonia and Torres-Coronas, Teresa (2020). “Substance Use among Spanish Adolescents: The Information Paradox”. International Journal Environmental. Research and Public Health, 17: 627. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17020627
Berge, Jonas; Sundell, Knut; Öjehagen, Agneta and Håkansson, Anders (2016). “Role of Parenting Styles in Adolescent Substance Use: Results from a Swedish Longitudinal Cohort Study”. BMJ Open, 6(1): e008979. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2015-008979
Bisset, Sherri; Markham, Wolfgang A. and Aveyard, Paul (2007). “School Culture as an Influencing Factor on Youth Substance Use”. Journal of epidemiology and community health, 61(6): 485. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.048157
Bousoño, Matilde; Al-Halabi, Susana; Buron, Patricia; Garrido, Marlen; Díaz-Mesa, Eva; Galván, Gonzalo; García-Álvarez, Leticia; Velasco, Ángela; Rodríguez-Revuelta, Julia; Wasserman, Camilla; Carli, Vladimir; Hove, Christina; Sarchiapone, Marco; Wasserman, Danuta; Bousoño, Manuel; García-Portilla, M.ª Paz; Iglesias, Celso; Saiz, Pilar A. and Bobes, Julio (2021). “Alcohol Use and Risk Factors for Self-harm Behavior in Spanish Adolescents”. Adicciones, 33(1). doi: 10.20882/adicciones.1239
Brown, Sandra A. (2008). Prevalence of Alcohol and Drug Involvement during Childhood and Adolescence. In: T. P. Beauchaine and S. P. Hinshaw (eds.). Child and adolescent psychopathology (pp. 405-444). Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc.
Burk, William J.; Vorst, Haske van der; Kerr, Margaret and Stattin, Hakan (2012). “Alcohol Intoxication Frequency and Friendship Dynamics: Selection and Socialization in Early, Middle- and Late-adolescent Peer Networks”. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 73(1): 89-98. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.89
Cabrera, Víctor; Ordóñez, Ana; González, Inés; Civantos, Victoria; Moriano, Juan A. and Lloret, Daniel (2022). “Evaluación de la eficacia de un programa de prevención escolar del consumo de alcohol y cannabis entre adolescentes”. Revista Española de Salud Pública, 96(17): 18. Available at: RS96C_202201004.pdf (sanidad.gob.es), access October 9, 2023.
Calafat, Amador; Juan, Montserrat; Becoña, Elisardo; Castillo, Agurtzane; Fernández, Cesáreo; Franco, Marta; Pereiro, César and Ros, Marga (2005). “El consumo de alcohol en la lógica del botellón”. Adicciones, 17(3): 193-202. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.368
Calero, Alejandra D.; Barreyro, Juan P.; Formoso, Jessica and Injoque-Ricle, Irene (2022). “Necesidad de pertenencia al grupo de pares y consumo de alcohol en la adolescencia”. Psicodebate: psicología, cultura y sociedad, 22(2): 47-59. doi: 10.18682/pd.v22i2.5145
Cano, Albert Julià; Escapa Solanas, Sandra; Marí-Klose, Marga and Marí-Klose, Pau (2012). “Factores de riesgo psicosociales en el consumo de tabaco de los adolescentes: estados de ánimo negativos, grupo de iguales y estilos parentales”. Adicciones, 24(4): 309-317.
Charro-Baena, Belén; Meneses, Carmen; Caperos, José M.; Prieto, María and Uroz, Jorge (2019). “The Role of Religion and Religiosity in Alcohol Consumption in Adolescents in Spain”. Journal of Religion and Health, 58: 1477-1487. doi: 10.1007/s10943-018-0694-z
Córdoba, Rodrigo; Camarelles, Francisco y San José, Joaquín (2017). «Posicionamiento sobre el consumo de alcohol semFYC». Atención Primaria, 49(9): 505-507. doi: 10.1016/j.aprim.2017.10.001
Cortés, M.ª Teresa; Espejo, Begoña and Giménez, José A. (2007). “Características que definen el fenómeno del botellón en universitarios y adolescentes”. Adicciones, 19(4): 357-372. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.295
Costello, E. Jane; Sung, Minje; Worthman, Carol and Angold, Adrian (2007). “Pubertal Maturation and the Development of Alcohol Use and Abuse”. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 88(1): 550-559. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.009
Cruz-Salmerón, Víctor Hugo; Martínez-Martínez, Martha L.; Garibay-López, Leticia and Camacho-Calderón, Nicolás (2011). “Comparación del perfil de funcionamiento familiar en los adolescentes con y sin drogadicción de un colegio de bachilleres”. Atención Primaria, 43(2): 89-94. doi: 10.1016/j.aprim.2010.04.009
De Looze Margaretha E.; Dorsselaer, Saskia A. F. M. van; Monshouwer, Karin and Vollebergh, Wilma A. M. (2017). “Trends in Adolescent Alcohol Use in the Netherlands, 1992-2015: Differences across Sociodemographic Groups and Links with Strict Parental Rule-setting”. International Journal of Drug Policy, 50: 90-101. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2017.09.013
Fuentes, María C.; Alarcón, Antonio; García, Fernando and Gracia, Enrique (2015). “Consumo de alcohol, tabaco, cannabis y otras drogas en la adolescencia: efectos de la familia y el barrio”. Anales de Psicología, 31(3): 1000-1007. doi: 10.6018/analesps.31.3.183491
García-Barba, Marta; Giménez-García, Cristina; Castro-Calvo, Jesús; Nebot, Juan, E. and Ballester-Arnal, Rafael (2018). “¿Existe relación entre el consumo de alcohol de los padres y el de los adolescentes?”. International Journal of Developmental and Educational Psychology, 2(1): 229-239. Available at: https://www.redalyc.org/journal/3498/349856003025/349856003025.pdf, access December 4, 2023.
García del Castillo, José A.; García del Castillo-López, Álvaro and López-Sánchez, Carmen (2014). La percepción de riesgo en la prevención del consumo de alcohol. In: M. T. Laespada Martínez (ed.). Consumo de alcohol en jóvenes y adolescentes: una mirada ecológica, (pp. 227-248). Bilbao: Universidad de Deusto.
Gommans, Rob; Müller, Christoph M.; Stevens, Gonneke W. J. M.; Cillessen, Antonius H. N. and Ter Bogt, Tom F. M. (2017). “Individual Popularity, Peer Group Popularity Composition and Adolescents’ Alcohol Consumption”. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46(8): 1716-1726. doi: 10.1007/s10964-016-0611-2
Gray, Kevin M. and Squeglia, Lindsay M. (2018). “Research Review: What Have We Learned about Adolescent Substance Use?”. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(6): 618-627. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12783
Gual, Antoni and Colom, Joan (1997). “Why Has Alcohol Consumption Declined in Countries of Southern Europe?”. Addiction, 92: S21-S31. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1997.tb03392.x
Guo, Hui; Reeder, Anthony I.; McGee, Rob and Darling, Helen (2011). “Adolescents’ Leisure Activities, Parental Monitoring and Cigarette Smoking - A Cross-sectional Study”. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy, 6(12). doi: 10.1186/1747-597X-6-12
Hair, Joseph and Alamer, Abdullah (2022). “Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) in Second Language and Education research: Guidelines Using an Applied Example”. Research Methods in Applied Linguistics, 1(3): 100027. doi: 10.1016/j.rmal.2022.100027
Helmer, Stefanie. M.; Burkhart, Gregor; Matias, João; Buck, Christoph; Engling Cardoso, Feline Engling and Vicente Julian (2021). “«Tell Me How Much Your Friends Consume» –Personal, Behavioral, Social, and Attitudinal Factors Associated with Alcohol and Cannabis Use among European School Students”. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(4):1684. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18041684
Jiménez-Iglesias, Antonia; Moreno, Carmen; Rivera, Francisco and García-Moya, Irene (2013). “The Role of the Family in Promoting Responsible Substance Use in Adolescence”. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22: 585-602. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9737-y
King, Pamela E. and Roeser, Robert W. (2009). 13 Religion and Spirituality in Adolescent Development. In: R. M. Lerner and L. Steinberg (eds.). Handbook of Adolescent Psychology: Individual bases of Adolescent Development (pp. 435-478). New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons Inc. doi: 10.1002/9780470479193.adlpsy001014
Lapsley, Daniel K. and Hill, Patrick L. (2010). “Subjective Invulnerability, Optimism Bias and Adjustment in Emerging Adulthood”. Journal of Youth and Adolescents, 39: 847-857. doi: 10.1007/s10964-009-9409-9
Leal-López, Eva; Sánchez-Queija, Inmaculada; Rivera, Francisco and Moreno, Carmen (2021a). “Tendencias en el consumo de alcohol en adolescentes escolarizados en España (2010-2018)”. Gaceta Sanitaria, 35(1): 35-41. doi: 10.1016/j.gaceta.2019.07.011.
Leal-López, Eva; Sánchez-Queija, Inmaculada; Vieno, Alessio; Currie, Dorothy; Torsheim, Torbjorn; Pavlova, Daria; Moreno-Maldonado, Concepción; De Clercq, Bart and Inchley, Joanna (2021b). “Cross-national Time Trends in Adolescent Alcohol Use from 2002 to 2014”. European journal of public health, 31(4): 859-866. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckab024
López-Larrosa, Silvia and Rodríguez-Arias, José Luis (2010). “Factores de riesgo y de protección en el consumo de drogas en adolescentes y diferencias según edad y sexo”. Psicothema, 22(4): 568-573. Available at: http://psicothema.com/pii?pii=3767´, access October 21, 2023.
Martínez-Lorca, Manuela and Alonso-Sanz, Carlos (2003). “Búsqueda de sensaciones, autoconcepto, asertividad y consumo de drogas. Existe relación?”. Adicciones, 15(2): 145-158. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.438
Martínez-Oró, David Pere and Romaní Alfonso, Oriol (2016). “Els danys de les polítiques prohibionistes en l’àmbit de les drogues”. Quaderns-e de l’Institut Català d’Antropologia, 21(1): 33-49.
Megías, Eusebio; Elzo, Javier; Rodríguez, Elena; Megías, Ignacio and Navarro, José (2006). Jóvenes, Valores, Drogas. Madrid: FAD. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.3670472
Mehanović, Emina; Vigna-Taglianti, Federica; Faggiano, Fabrizio and Galanti, Maria Rosaria (2022). “Does Parental Permissiveness toward Cigarette Smoking and Alcohol Use Influence Illicit Drug Use among Adolescents? A Longitudinal Study in Seven European Countries”. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 57: 173-181. doi: 10.1007/s00127-021-02118-5
Melguizo-Ibáñez, Eduardo; González-Valero, Gabriel; Badicu Georgian; Clemente, Filipe Manuel; Filipa, Ana and Puertas-Molero, P. (2023). “An Explanatory Model of Violent Behavior, Self-Concept, and Alcohol, Tobacco, and Cannabis Consumption in Secondary Education Students”. BioMed Research International, 1971858. doi: 10.1155/2023/1971858
Méndez Garrido, Juan M. and Azaustre Lorenzo M.ª Carmen (2016). “El consumo de alcohol en universitarios. Estudio de las relaciones entre las causas y los efectos negativos”. Revista Complutense de Educación, 28(3): 689-704. doi: 10.5209/rev_RCED.2017.v28.n3.49725
Mezquita, Laura; Maestre, Emma; Mestre, Helena; Viñas, Marta; Moya, Jorge and Ortet, Generós (2006). “Relación entre personalidad y consumo de alcohol en adolescentes españoles y escoceses”. Castelló: Forum de Recerca, 11: 2-12. Available at: https://repositori.uji.es/items/4c0338b7-4bec-4528-9860-9562fe93e48f, access October 17, 2023.
Mietzel, Gerd (2005). Claves de la psicología evolutiva: Infancia y juventud. Barcelona: Herder.
Moral-Jiménez, María de la Villa; Rodríguez-Díaz, Francisco J. and Sirvent-Ruiz, Carlos (2005). “Motivadores de consumo de alcohol en adolescentes: análisis de diferencias inter-género y propuesta de un continuum etiológico”. Adicciones, 17(2): 105-120. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.376
Musitu, Gonzalo (2014). “¿Por qué los adolescentes tienen una baja percepción de riesgos respecto del consumo de alcohol? La visión de los expertos”. Revista del Centro Reina Sofía sobre Adolescencia y Juventud, 1: 55-73. Available at: https://www.centroreinasofia.org/publicacion/metamorfosis-1/, access September 12, 2023.
Nawi, Azmawati M.; Ismail, Rozmi; Ibrahim, Fauziah; Hassan, Mohd R.; Manaf, Mohd R. A.; Amit, Noh; Ibrahim, Norhayati and Shafurdin, Nurul S. (2021). “Risk and Protective Factors of Drug Abuse among Adolescents: A Systematic Review”. BMC public health, 21(1): 1-15. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-11906-2
Neighbors, Clayton; Young, Chelsie M.; Krieger, Heather and Tackett, Jennifer L. (2016). Social Influence, Pressure, and Norms: Vulnerability for Substance Use in Adolescents. In: C. E. Kopetz and C. W. Lejuez (eds.). Addictions: A social psychological perspective (pp. 170-198). London: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Novak, Miranda; Maglica, Toni and Radetic Paic, Mirjana (2022). “School, Family, and Peer Predictors of Adolescent Alcohol and Marijuana Use”. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy, 30(5): 486-496. doi: 10.1080/09687637.2022.2073869
OEDA (Observatorio Español de las Drogas y las Adicciones) (2022). Alcohol, tabaco y drogas ilegales en España. Madrid: Ministerio de Sanidad. Delegación del Gobierno para el Plan Nacional sobre Drogas.
OEDA (Observatorio Español de las Drogas y las Adicciones) (2023). Alcohol, tabaco y drogas ilegales en España. Madrid: Ministerio de Sanidad. Delegación del Gobierno para el Plan Nacional sobre Drogas.
Ortega-García, Juan A.; Tellerías, Lydia; Ferrís-Tortajada, Josep; Boldo, Elena; Campillo-López, Ferran; Van den Hazel, Peter; Cortés-Arancibia, Sandra; Ramis, Rebeca; Gaioli, Marisa; Monroy-Torres, Rebeca; Farias-Guardia, Constanza; Borras, Mirta; Yohannessen, Karla; García-Noriega, Marcelino; Cáceres-Álvarez, Alberto; Jaimes-Vega, Diana C.; Cordero-Rizo, Marcia; López-Hernández, Fernando and Claudio, Luz (2019). “Amenazas, desafíos y oportunidades para la salud medioambiental pediátrica en Europa, América Latina y el Caribe”. Anales de Pediatría, 90(2): 124e1-124e11. doi: 10.1016/j.anpedi.2018.11.015
Patrick, Megan E.; Peterson, Sarah J.; Terry-McElrath, Yvonne M.; Rogan, Shanna Elaine B. and Solberg, Marvin A. (2024). “Trends in Coping Reasons for Marijuana use Among U.S. Adolescents from 2016 to 2022”. Addictive Behaviors, 148: 107845. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2023.107845
Perelló, Miguel J.; Llorens, Noelia and Tortajada, Silvia (2008). “Influencia de los estilos educativos paternos en el consumo de drogas en adolescentes”. Revista Española de Drogodependencias, 33(4): 288-299.
Pérez de Albéniz-Garrote, Gloria; Medina-Gómez, Begoña and Rubio-Rubio, Laura (2019). “Influencia de la impulsividad y de la búsqueda de sensaciones en el consumo precoz de Cannabis. Diferencias de género y orientaciones para la prevención”. Revista Española de Orientación y Psicopedagogía, 30(1): 27-40. doi: 10.5944/reop.vol.30.num.1.2019.25192
Pérez-Milena, Alejandro; Martínez-Fernández, M.ª Luz; Redondo-Olmedilla, Manuel; Álvarez Nieto, Carmen, Jiménez Pulido, Idoia and Mesa-Gallardo, Inmaculada (2012). “Motivaciones para el consumo de tabaco entre los adolescentes de un instituto urbano”. Gaceta Sanitaria, 26(1).
Pons, Javier and Buelga, Sofía (2011). “Factores Asociados al Consumo Juvenil de Alcohol: Una Revisión desde una Perspectiva Psicosocial y Ecológica”. Psychosocial Intervention, 20(1): 75-94. Available at: https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=179818575008
Prignitz, Maren; Banaschewski, Tobias; Bokde, Arun L. W.; Desrivières, Sylvane; Grigis, Antoine; Garavan, Hugh; Gowland, Penny; Heinz, Andreas; Martinot, Jean-Luc.; Paillère, Marie-Laure; Artigues, Eric; Papadopoulos, Dimitri; Poustka, Luise; Hohmann, Sarah; Fröhner, Julianne H.; Robinson, Lauren; Smolka, Michael N.; Walter, Henrik; Winterer, Jeanne M.; Whelan, Robert et al. (2023). “The Role of Empathy in Alcohol Use of Bullying Perpetrators and Victims: Lower Per-sonal Empathic Distress Makes Male Perpetrators of Bullying More Vulnerable to Alcohol Use”. International Journal Environmental. Research and Public Health, 20: e6286. Available at: https://dx.doi.org/10.5093/in2011v20n1a7, access January 29, 2024.
Rubio, Gabriel; Nieto, Pedro L.; Martín, Isabel A.; Rubio, Enrique and Martínez, Mario (2022). Los problemas del consumo de alcohol en jóvenes. In: C. Noriega, G. Pérez-Rojo and M. Isabel Carretero (eds.). Las adicciones en los adolescentes (pp. 67-78). Madrid: Editorial Dykinson.
Salamó-Avellaneda, Anna; Gras-Pérez, M.ª Eugenia and Font-Mayolas, Silvia (2010). “Patrones de consumo de alcohol en la adolescencia”. Psicothema, 22(2): 189-195. Available at: https://www.psicothema.com/pii?pii=3714, access November 22, 2023.
Sánchez-Aragón, Anna; Valls-Fonayet, Francesc; Pastor-Gosálbez, Inmaculada; Anleu-Hernández, Claudia María and Belzunegui-Eraso, Ángel (2024). “Motivaciones para el consumo de alcohol en adolescentes escolarizados: análisis de grupos de discusión”. Atención Primaria, 56: 102931. doi: 10.1016/j.aprim.2024.102931
Sánchez-Quejica, Inmaculada; Moreno, Carmen; Rivera, Francisco and Ramos, Pilar (2015). “Tendencias en el consumo de alcohol en los adolescentes escolarizados españoles a lo largo de la primera década del siglo XXI”. Gaceta Sanitaria, 29(3): 184-189. doi: 10.1016/j.gaceta.2015.01.004
Scheffels, Janne; Brunborg, Geir S.; Bilgrei, O. R.; Tokle, Rikke; Burdzovic, Jasmina and Buvik, Kristin (2023). “The Ambivalence of Alcohol Expectancies: A Longitudinal Mixed Methods Study among 12-to-18-year-old Adolescents”. Journal of Adolescent Research, 0(0). doi: 10.1177/07435584221150909
Smit, Koen; Voogt, Carmen; Otten, Roy; Kleinjan, Marloes and Kuntsche, Emmanuel (2022). “Why Adolescents Engage in Early Alcohol Use: A study of Drinking Motives”. Clinical Psychology Review, 25(7): 841-861. doi: 10.1037/pha0000383
Song, Eu-Young; Smiler, Andrew P.; Wagoner, Kimberly G. and Wolfson, Mark (2012). “Everyone Says It’s Ok: Adolescents’ Perceptions of Peer, parent, and Community Alcohol Norms, Alcohol Consumption, and Alcohol-related Consequences”. Substance Use & Misuse, 47(1): 86-98. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2011.629704
Soriano-Sánchez, José and Jiménez-Vázquez, David (2022). “Predictores del consumo de alcohol en adolescentes: una revisión sistemática de estudios transversales”. Revista Estudios Psicológicos, 2(4): 73-86. doi: 10.35622/j.rep.2022.04.006
Suárez, Cristian; Moral, Gonzalo del; Musitu, Gonzalo; Sánchez, Juan Carlos and John, Bev (2014). “Eficacia de las políticas institucionales de prevención del consumo de alcohol en adolescentes: la opinión de expertos y adolescentes”. Atención Primaria, 46: 326-335. doi: 10.1016/j.aprim.2013.11.005
Suites, Daniel B. (1957). “Use of Dummy Variables in Regression Equations”. Journal of the American Statistical Association 52(280): 548-551 (republished online in 2012). doi: 10.2307/2281705
Talbott, Laura L.; Martin, Ryan J.; Usdan, Stuart L.; Leeper, James D.; Umstattd, Rénee; Cremeens, Jennifer L. and Geiger, Brian F. (2008). “Drinking Likelihood, Alcohol Problems, and Peer Influence among First-year College Students”. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 34: 433-440. doi: 10.1080/00952990802122655
Tong, Ming; Ziplow Jason L.; Princess, Mark and De la Monte, Suzanne M. (2022). “Dietary Soy Prevents Alcohol-Mediated Neurocognitive Dysfunction and Associated Impairments in Brain Insulin Pathway Signaling in an Adolescent Rat Model”. Biomolecules, 12(5): e676. doi: 10.3390/biom12050676
Urrutia-Pereira, Marilyn; Soléb, Dirceu; Chong, Herberto José; Badellinod, Héctor; Acoste, V., Castro-Almarales, Raúl Lázaro; León, M.G.; Avalos, M. M.; Fernández, Carmen C.; Sisul-Alvariza, Juan Carlos, Oliano, Vinícius Jardim and Rinelli, Pietro N. (2019). “Youth Tobacco Use in Latin America: What Is the Real Extent of the Problem?”. Allergologia et Immunopathologia, 47(4): 328-335. doi: 10.1016/j.aller.2018.09.010
Villanueva, Víctor J. and Duque, M.ª Aranzazu (2022). Alcohol, tabaco y cannabis en jóvenes. In: EFYPAF. La promoción de comportamientos saludables desde los centros educativos. Ejemplos de proyectos de intervención eficaces (pp. 87-96). Zaragoza: Servicio de publicaciones de la Universidad de Zaragoza.
Wellman, Robert J.; Sabiston, Catherine M. and Morgenstern, Matthis (2022). “Depressive Symptoms, Alcohol Beliefs and Heavy Episodic Drinking in Adolescents”. Children, 9(1): 103. doi: 10.3390/children9010103
Wray-Lake, Laura; Crouter, Ann C. and McHale, Susan M. (2010). “Developmental Patterns in Decision-making Autonomy across Middle Childhood and Adolescence: European American parents’ perspectives”. Child Development, 81(2): 636-651. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01420.x
1 Financing: The study underlying this work is the National Plan R&D+I 2019 of the Ministry of Science and Innovation. The title of the study is “Prevención del consumo de drogas y del juego en adolescentes: la paradoja de la información. El caso de Tarragona” (Prevention of drug consumption and gambling in adolescents: the information paradox. The Tarragon case) Code: PID2019-104310RB-C21.
Ethical declaration: 1) participants and their legal guardians were informed about the study and procedures; 2) the anonymity of the data collected was guaranteed; 3) the study was conducted with the support of the City Hall of Tarragona through its Addiction Prevention Committee, and the Department of Education of the regional government of Catalonia; 4) participation in the questionnaire was voluntary for the adolescents after obtaining the permit from the school administration and their legal guardians.
The study has been carried out in accordance with the provisions of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Universitat Rovira i Virgili (CEIPSA-2021-PDR-39).
2 Spain’s National Plan on Drugs defines binge drinking as the consumption of five or more alcoholic drinks (in the case of men) or four or more (in the case of women) within a short period of time, usually around two hours.
List 1. Dependent variables in the analysis
Name of the dependent variable |
Description of the variable |
Response categories |
CONSUMPTION |
Number of times consuming alcohol over the past 30 days. |
0=“Never”, 1=“Once or twice”, 2=“3 to 9 times”, 3=“Between 10 and 20 times” and 4=“More than 20 times”. |
DRUNKENNESS |
Number of times that the adolescent declares to have gotten drunk over the past 30 days. |
Idem to CONSUMPTION |
BINGE DRINKING (BINGE) |
Number of times that the adolescent declares to have consumed alcohol in the binge drinking modality over the past 30 days. |
Idem to CONSUMPTION |
Source: Author’s own creation.
List 2. Independent variables in the analysis
Name of the independent variable |
Description of the variable |
Response categories |
SEX |
Sex of the interviewed person |
1=“Girl”; 0=“Boy”; 2=“Prefer not to respond”. |
AGE |
Age in years |
Re-coded as: 1=“17 or older”; 0=“Under 17”. |
CU_RULES (Rule compliance) |
CU_RULES_1: the majority of the rules can be broken if they are inconvenient. CU_RULES_2: I follow the rules I want to follow. CU_RULES_3: it is hard to trust something because everything changes. CU_RULES_4: in fact, no one knows what is expected of him/her in life. CU_RULES_5: you cannot be sure of anything in life. CU_RULES_6: sometimes, it is necessary to break the rules to be successful. CU_RULES_7: following the rules does not guarantee success. |
1=“Totally disagree”; 2=“Strongly disagree”; 3=“Neither agree nor disagree”; 4=“Strongly agree”; 5=“Totally agree”. |
IRRITABILITY |
IRRITABILITY_1: I have become easily annoyed or irritated. IRRITABILITY_2: I have had outbursts of anger that I have not been able to control. IRRITABILITY_3: I have wanted to break or damage things. IRRITABILITY_4: I have fought with someone. IRRITABILITY_5: I have yelled or thrown things at someone. |
1=“Almost never”; 2=“Rarely”; 3=“Sometimes”; 4=“Often”; 5=“Almost always”. |
SUPPORT_SCHOOL (Support from the school) |
SUPPORT_SCHOOL_1: adults at my school care about me. SUPPORT_SCHOOL_2: I have friends at my school who care about me. SUPPORT_SCHOOL_3: students at my school are friendly to one another. SUPPORT_SCHOOL_4: my school helps me to achieve the objectives that I care about. SUPPORT_SCHOOL_5: I like to participate in activities at my school. |
1=“Totally disagree”; 2=“ Strongly disagree”; 3=“Neither agree nor disagree”; 4=“Strongly agree”; 5=“Totally agree”. |
CONTROL_P (Parental control) |
My parents… CONTROL_P_1: they think it is very important that my studies go well. CONTROL_P_2: they establish clear rules about what I can do at home. CONTROL_P_3: they establish clear rules about what I can do outside of my house. CONTROL_P_4: they establish clear rules about when I have to be home at night. CONTROL_P_5: they know who I am with at night. CONTROL_P_6: they know where I am at night. CONTROL_P_7: they know my friends. CONTROL_P_8: they know the parents of my friends. |
1=“Does not apply to me at all”; 2=“Does not apply well to me”; 3=“Applies to me a good deal”; 4=“Applies very well to me”. |
INFL_PEERS (Peer influence) |
INFL_PEERS_1: sometimes it is necessary to smoke cigarettes so that I am not left out of my group of peers. INFL_PEERS_2: sometimes it is necessary to drink alcohol to avoid being left out of my peer group. INFL_PEERS_3: sometimes it is necessary to smoke weed to avoid being left out of my peer group. INFL_PEERS_4: sometimes it is necessary to skip class to avoid being left out of my peer group. |
1=“Totally disagree”; 2=“Strongly disagree”; 3=“Neither agree nor disagree”; 4=“Strongly agree”; 5=“Totally agree”. |
Religious affiliation |
Identification with a certain religion or with none. |
1=“Catholic”; 2=“Evangelical”; 3=“Muslim”; 4=“Orthodox”; 5=“Other religion”; 6=“I do not identify with any religion”. |
LEVEL_INF (Level of information) |
Do you consider yourself well informed about the consequences of substance use? |
1=“Completely uninformed”; 2=“Largely uninformed”; 3=“Neither informed nor uninformed”; 4=“Largely informed”; 5=“Completely informed”. LEVEL_INF will ultimately be the normalized value of the responses such that LEVEL_INF=“(IQ9-1)/4”. |
INTERNET |
Information source. |
1=“Yes”; 0=“No”. |
SCHOOL |
Information source. |
Idem |
PARENTS |
Information source. |
Idem |
SIBLINGS |
Information source. |
Idem |
FRIENDS |
Information source. |
Idem |
MEDIA |
Information source. |
Idem |
Source: Author’s own creation.
List 2. Independent variables in the analysis (Continuation)
List 2. Independent variables in the analysis (Continuation)
Table 1. Response percentages of the variables used in the analysis (%)
Variables explained |
Never |
Between 1 |
Between 3 |
Between 10 and 20 times |
More than 20 times |
NC |
CONSUMPTION |
58.2 |
28.6 |
8.1 |
0.6 |
1.1 |
3.4 |
DRUNKENNESS |
78.7 |
14.7 |
1.6 |
0.1 |
0.9 |
3.9 |
BINGE DRINKING |
73.2 |
17.3 |
3.9 |
0.4 |
0.9 |
4.3 |
Explanatory variables |
||||||
SEX Female (46.5 %) Male (51.2 %) NC/other (2.3 %) |
AGE ≥17 years (43.8 %) <17 years (53.6 %) NC (2.6 %) |
|||||
IQ3: Regulatory compliance |
Totally disagree |
Strongly disagree |
Neither agree nor disagree |
Strongly agree |
Totally agree |
NC |
CU_RULES_1 |
13.9 |
19.6 |
34.9 |
13.2 |
8.7 |
9.7 |
CU_RULES_2 |
11.1 |
14.8 |
22.5 |
27.8 |
16.2 |
7.6 |
CU_RULES_3 |
4.8 |
7.4 |
29.5 |
29.4 |
19.6 |
9.4 |
CU_RULES_4 |
4.4 |
7.2 |
23.9 |
30.9 |
24.9 |
8.6 |
CU_RULES_5 |
5.6 |
6.7 |
18.6 |
31.6 |
30.4 |
7.2 |
CU_RULES_6 |
8.3 |
9.9 |
29.9 |
23.6 |
20.4 |
7.9 |
CU_RULES_7 |
4.8 |
6.7 |
26.1 |
25.3 |
29.3 |
7.8 |
IQ4: Irritability |
Almost never |
Rarely |
Sometimes |
Often |
Almost always |
NC |
IRRITABILITY_1 |
13.0 |
20.6 |
26.8 |
21.7 |
13.4 |
4.5 |
IRRITABILITY_2 |
33.0 |
26.7 |
16.5 |
11.8 |
7.4 |
4.7 |
IRRITABILITY_3 |
42.2 |
24.2 |
13.8 |
8.3 |
6.9 |
4.7 |
IRRITABILITY_4 |
51.3 |
21.4 |
13.2 |
4.1 |
5.0 |
4.9 |
IRRITABILITY_5 |
49.7 |
21.4 |
13.2 |
5.7 |
5.0 |
5.1 |
IQ5: Support of the school |
Totally disagree |
Strongly disagree |
Neither agree nor disagree |
Strongly agree |
Totally agree |
NC |
SUPPORT_SCHOOL_1 |
4.5 |
8.9 |
30.1 |
35.7 |
17.4 |
3.4 |
SUPPORT_SCHOOL_2 |
2.1 |
3.1 |
10.8 |
33.7 |
47.1 |
3.1 |
SUPPORT_SCHOOL _3 |
5.8 |
9.7 |
30.9 |
32.8 |
17.6 |
3.1 |
SUPPORT_SCHOOL _4 |
6.1 |
11.6 |
30.8 |
32.1 |
15.3 |
4.1 |
SUPPORT_SCHOOL _5 |
7.4 |
10.6 |
34.6 |
28.1 |
15.5 |
3.8 |
IQ6: Control parental |
Absolutely does not apply to me |
Does not apply well to me |
Applies somewhat to me |
Applies very well to me |
NC |
|
CONTROL_P_1 |
0.6 |
2.8 |
26.6 |
65.5 |
4.6 |
|
CONTROL_P_2 |
4.2 |
8.7 |
41.9 |
38.4 |
6.8 |
|
CONTROL_P_3 |
4.9 |
10.4 |
39.8 |
37.6 |
7.2 |
|
CONTROL_P_4 |
7.3 |
14.1 |
36.2 |
34.3 |
8.2 |
|
CONTROL_P_5 |
3.5 |
6.2 |
23.1 |
59.1 |
8.1 |
|
CONTROL_P_6 |
2.9 |
4.4 |
21.9 |
63.1 |
7.6 |
|
CONTROL_P_7 |
3.3 |
6.9 |
26.3 |
57.8 |
5.7 |
|
CONTROL_P_8 |
10.6 |
17.3 |
34.9 |
29.0 |
8.1 |
|
IQ7: Influence of peers |
Totally disagree |
Strongly disagree |
Neither agree nor disagree |
Strongly agree |
Totally agree |
NC |
INFL_PEERS_1 |
83.1 |
6.4 |
3.5 |
1.2 |
1.8 |
4.1 |
INFL_PEERS_2 |
76.4 |
9.9 |
6.1 |
1.9 |
1.3 |
4.4 |
INFL_PEERS_3 |
85.0 |
4.4 |
3.3 |
1.4 |
1.5 |
4.4 |
INFL_PEERS_4 |
80.6 |
8.3 |
4.1 |
0.5 |
1.8 |
4.7 |
IQ8: Religion |
||||||
1. Catholic (34.6 %) 2. Evangelist (3.1 %) 3. Islam (10.3 %) 4. Orthodox (2.9 %) |
5. Other (1.8 %) 6. I do not identify with any religion (44.6 %) 7. NC (2.7 %) |
|||||
IQ9: LEVEL_INF (Level of information) |
Completely uninformed |
Quite uninformed |
Neither informed nor uninformed |
Very informed |
Completely informed |
NC |
2.2 |
3.2 |
10.5 |
36.7 |
42.1 |
5.3 |
|
My information on substance use comes from: |
Si |
No |
NC |
|||
Internet |
65.9 |
27.2 |
6.9 |
|||
School |
66.3 |
26.8 |
6.9 |
|||
Parents/legal guardians |
62.4 |
29.9 |
7.7 |
|||
Siblings |
23.4 |
68.3 |
8.3 |
|||
Peers and friends |
44.7 |
47.1 |
8.2 |
|||
Mass media |
55.1 |
37.0 |
7.9 |
Notes: (a) The quantities appear as percentages.
Source: Author’s own creation.
Table 1. Response percentages of the variables used in the analysis (%) (Continuation)
Table 1. Response percentages of the variables used in the analysis (%) (Continuation)
TablE 2. Spearman correlations (ρ) between CONSUMPTION and DRUNKENNESS and BINGE DRINKING with the explanatory variables (factors)
Factors |
Alcohol consumption modalities |
||
Individual |
CONSUMPTION |
DRUNKENNESS |
BINGE DRINKING |
SEX |
-0.016 (0.563) |
-0.053 (0.055) |
0.002 (0.942) |
AGE |
0.196** (<0.001) |
0.139** (<0.001) |
0.111** (<0.001) |
CU_RULES |
0.121** (<0.001) |
0.065* (0.033) |
0.110** (0.001) |
IRRITABILITY |
0.148** (<0.001) |
0.197** (<0.001) |
0.131** (<0.001) |
Setting |
|||
SUPPORT_SCHOOL |
-0.039 (0.181) |
-0.019 (0.509) |
-0.014 (0.638) |
CONTROL_P |
-0.002 (0.942) |
-0.064* (0.023) |
-0.052 (0.067) |
INFL_PEERS |
0.201** (<0.001) |
0.197** (<0.001) |
0.222** (<0.001) |
ISLAM |
-0.221** (<0.001) |
-0.118** (<0.001) |
-0.133** (<0.001) |
Information |
|||
LEVEL_INF |
0.009 (0.745) |
0.013 (0.638) |
0.026 (0.386) |
INTERNET |
0.82* (0.004) |
0.106** (<0.001) |
0.051 (0.078) |
SCHOOL |
-0.031 (0.286) |
-0.048 (0.095) |
-0.034 (0.247) |
PARENTS |
-0.022 (0.447) |
-0.053 (0.067) |
-0.017 (0.566) |
SIBLINGS |
0.074* (0.011) |
0.084** (0.004) |
0.088** (0.003) |
PEERS |
0.146** (<0.001) |
0.158** (<0.001) |
0.165** (<0.001) |
MEDIA |
0.006 (0.835) |
-0.007 (0.815) |
-0.047 (0.108) |
Note: * and ** represent statistical significance at 5 % and 1 %, respectively. The p-values are shown between parentheses.
Source: Author’s own creation.
Table 3. Results of ordered logistic regressions on CONSUMPTION
Factors |
Model without information variables |
Model with information variables |
||||
Individual |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
SEX |
1.044 |
0.599 |
[0.889, 1.226] |
1.029 |
0.746 |
[0.867, 1.220] |
AGE |
1.698** |
<0.001 |
[1.453, 1.984] |
1.690** |
<0.0001 |
[1.434, 1.992] |
CU_RULES |
1.189** |
<0.001 |
[1.094, 1.292] |
1.185** |
0.000 |
[1.086, 1.294] |
IRRITABILITY |
1.126** |
0.004 |
[1.039, 1.221] |
1.123** |
0.007 |
[1.032, 1.222] |
Environment |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
SCHOOL SUPPORT |
0.939 |
0.133 |
[0.865, 1.019] |
0.924 |
0.075 |
[0.847, 1.008] |
P CONTROL |
1.070 |
0.258 |
[0.951, 1.205] |
0.966 |
0.603 |
[0.846, 1.102] |
INFL_PEERS |
1.282** |
<0.001 |
[1.188, 1.384] |
1.234** |
<0.001 |
[1.136, 1.342] |
ISLAM |
0.189** |
<0.001 |
[0.112, 0.318] |
0.134** |
<0.001 |
[0.068, 0.261] |
Information |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
INF LEVEL |
- |
- |
- |
1.291 |
0.195 |
[0.878, 1.899] |
INTERNET |
- |
- |
- |
1.042 |
0.679 |
[0.859, 1.263] |
SCHOOL CENTER |
- |
- |
- |
0.857 |
0.112 |
[0.708, 1.037] |
PARENTS |
- |
- |
- |
0.913 |
0.353 |
[0.754, 1.106] |
SIBLINGS |
- |
- |
- |
1.304** |
0.008 |
[1.073, 1.584] |
PEERS |
- |
- |
- |
1.422** |
<0.001 |
[1.197, 1.690] |
MEDIA |
- |
- |
- |
0.891 |
0.197 |
[0.747, 1.062] |
Pseudo-R2 |
9.73 |
Pseudo-R2 |
11.99 |
|||
Akaike |
1733.119 |
Akaike |
1585.893 |
|||
Schwartz |
1791.758 |
Schwartz |
1677.514 |
|||
Hannan-Quinn |
1755.429 |
Hannan-Quinn |
1620.859 |
|||
LR-ratio |
184.26** |
LR-ratio |
210.99** |
Note: * and ** represent statistical significance at a 5 % and 1 % level, respectively.
Source: Author’s own creation.
Table 4. Results of ordered logistic regressions on DRUNKENNESS
Factors |
Model without information variables |
Model with information variables |
||||
Individual |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
SEX |
0.873 |
0.177 |
[0.717, 1.063] |
0.903 |
0.342 |
[0.731, 1.115] |
AGE |
1.381** |
0.001 |
[1.141, 1.670] |
1.378** |
0.002 |
[1.124, 1.689] |
CU_RULES |
1.111* |
0.044 |
[1.003, 1.231] |
1.075 |
0.192 |
[0.964, 1.200] |
IRRITABILITY |
1.236** |
<0.001 |
[1.124, 1.360] |
1.246** |
<0.001 |
[1.126, 1.379] |
Environment |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
SCHOOL SUPPORT |
0.895* |
0.029 |
[0.810, 0.989] |
0.896* |
0.045 |
[0.804, 0.997] |
P CONTROL |
0.914 |
0.205 |
[0.795, 1.051] |
0.843* |
0.036 |
[0.719, 0.989] |
INFL_PEERS |
1.304** |
<0.001 |
[1.199, 1.418] |
1.240** |
<0.001 |
[1.130, 1.361] |
ISLAM |
0.350 |
<0.001 |
[0.201, 0.612] |
0.250** |
<0.001 |
[0.120, 0.520] |
Information |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
INF LEVEL |
- |
- |
- |
1.138 |
0.585 |
[0.715, 1.812] |
INTERNET |
- |
- |
- |
1.264 |
0.070 |
[0.981, 1.628] |
SCHOOL CENTER |
- |
- |
- |
0.853 |
0.179 |
[0.676, 1.075] |
PARENTS |
- |
- |
- |
0.882 |
0.297 |
[0.697, 1.117] |
SIBLINGS |
- |
- |
- |
1.399** |
0.005 |
[1.107, 1.767] |
PEERS |
- |
- |
- |
1.542** |
<0.001 |
[1.242, 1.915] |
MEDIA |
- |
- |
- |
0.774* |
0.022 |
[0.621, 0.964] |
Pseudo-R2 |
9.81 |
Pseudo-R2 |
13.61 |
|||
Akaike |
1029.339 |
Akaike |
926.0677 |
|||
Schwartz |
1087.953 |
Schwartz |
1017.731 |
|||
Hannan-Quinn |
1051.641 |
Hannan-Quinn |
961.0467 |
|||
LR-ratio |
109.46** |
LR-ratio |
139.92** |
Note: * and ** represent statistical significance at a 5 % and 1 % level, respectively.
Source: Author’s own creation.
Table 5. Results of ordered logistic regressions on BINGE
Factors |
Model without information variables |
Model with information variables |
||||
Individual |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
SEX |
0.995 |
0.957 |
[0.829, 1.194] |
0.983 |
0.861 |
[0.808, 1.195] |
AGE |
1.408** |
0.000 |
[1.180, 1.680] |
1.437** |
<0.001 |
[1.191, 1.735] |
CU_RULES |
1.203** |
0.000 |
[1.093, 1.324] |
1.183** |
0.001 |
[1.068, 1.309] |
IRRITABILITY |
1.121* |
0.013 |
[1.024, 1.227] |
1.126* |
0.014 |
[1.024, 1.239] |
Environment |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
SCHOOL SUPPORT |
0.921 |
0.082 |
[0.839, 1.011] |
0.914 |
0.076 |
[0.827, 1.009] |
P CONTROL |
0.957 |
0.505 |
[0.841, 1.089] |
0.859* |
0.040 |
[0.743, 0.993] |
INFL_PEERS |
1.353** |
<0.001 |
[1.248, 1.467] |
1.321** |
<0.001 |
[1.211, 1.442] |
ISLAM |
0.295** |
<0.001 |
[0.170, 0.514] |
0.212** |
<0.001 |
[0.103, 0.437] |
Information |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
OR |
p-value |
IC (95 %) |
INF LEVEL |
- |
- |
- |
1.462 |
0.093 |
[0.938, 2.278] |
INTERNET |
- |
- |
- |
1.053 |
0.648 |
[0.842, 1.318] |
SCHOOL CENTER |
- |
- |
- |
0.950 |
0.645 |
[0.765, 1.181] |
PARENTS |
- |
- |
- |
0.948 |
0.629 |
[0.762, 1.179] |
SIBLINGS |
- |
- |
- |
1.326* |
0.012 |
[1.065, 1.651] |
PEERS |
- |
- |
- |
1.621** |
<0.001 |
[1.327, 1.980] |
MEDIA |
- |
- |
- |
0.729** |
0.002 |
[0.596, 0.891] |
Pseudo-R2 |
9.17 |
Pseudo-R2 |
13.15 |
|||
Akaike |
1276.294 |
Akaike |
1154.472 |
|||
Schwartz |
1334.871 |
Schwartz |
1246.073 |
|||
Hannan-Quinn |
1298.585 |
Hannan-Quinn |
1189.432 |
|||
LR-ratio |
126.49** |
LR-ratio |
169.16** |
Note: * and ** represent statistical significance at a 5 % and 1 % level, respectively.
Source: Author’s own creation.
RECEPTION: February 23, 2024
REVIEW: August 01, 2024
ACCEPTANCE: October 11, 2024